EDR725
 StartSyllabusClassLibraryCommunicate
Help EDR725 : The Class : Participant Observation : Participant & Observer : Electronic Textbook

"Out Standing in the Field:" Essentials of Pariticipant Observation

 

Yeah, yeah -- bad joke, I know! I couldn't resist: it was the "punch line" on an adorable coffee mug given to me by a doctoral student colleague right after my prospectus hearing!

I. A Delicate "Balancing Act:" Participant & Observer

Although these two terms are indeed used jointly to characterize this particular method of qualitative data collection, they carry some subtle contradictions as well. Namely, here is the delicate balance to be maintained between the two:

 

 

Figure 1.

Tradeoffs of Being a "Participant" and "Observer"

Start thinking about what is involved in observation by opening and reading the link below.

On Observation

* The "participant part" implies an immersive experience in a real-world, "field-based" setting. As such it implies that the researcher is holistically committing his/her feelings, thoughts, emotions, etc. to that setting.

* On the other hand, the "observer part" is characterized by the scientific approach to knowledge. That implies objective, scientific, neutral, scholarly recording of these data or observations.

At first glance, it seems like a rather precarious balancing act, doesn't it?

* Go too far in the "participant" extreme and the researcher runs the risk of, as Miles and Huberman characterize it, "going native." He/she can in essence become "co-opted" by the situation, setting, key players, etc., to such a degree that scientific objectivity is lost. Michael Quinn Patton describes such a study. The researchers were observing and recording data on audience reactions to a crusade being conducted by the Rev. Dr. Billy Graham. At one point, towards the end of the crusade, two of the researchers put down their note pads and walked down the aisle to join those audience members who had accepted Dr. Graham's call.

* Go too far in the "observer" direction and you run the risk of doing what I call "overly sterile qualitative research!" By that I mean skimping on the "rich, thick, vivid description" that we have learned is the hallmark of qualitative research.

For some of us, this danger is admittedly a "spillover" from the old quantitative paradigm: say it concisely -- p-value of X, reject the null hypothesis, etc.The qualitative equivalent would be to record, "The support group meeting took place in a school," and think you have "adequately" characterized the setting. But think of it this way: would you and I necessarily conjure up the same mental picture when we read the word, "school?" You might picture a large room with desks in straight rows in a city surrounding. Someone else might envision a laboratory-type setting with round tables that seat at most 5 or 6 students, with available tactile learning materials. Someone else could easily imagine a home school environment. Plus, I hear that there are still some vestiges of the "one-room schoolhouse" in existence! Hope this gets the point across! and the resultant need for "rich, thick description!"

For other qualitative researchers, the danger here is in how the field notes are recorded -- "jumping to conclusions," e.g., "the group seemed friendly." Again, it comes down to providing detailed, vivid narrative as to the evidence for this. How did you arrive at that conclusion? Did they smile and make frequent eye contact with one another? Was there ready agreement and support for one another's comments and positions? Did they pull their chairs close together and sit in a circle as they talked? For that matter, how did they even greet one another as they came in? Handshakes? Hugs? Pats on the back or arm? And so on and so forth.

Stop a minute and read this anthropologists mixed-method (multi-method) field work study conducted in New Guinea.

Participation

Despite the danger in going to either extreme, it is possible to strike a balance and have the best of both worlds. That's what I've tried to show by the intersection of the 2 circles on pg. 2. I believe that balance lies in the following:

  1. Capitalizing on your opportunity for a "closeup view" by being in the field, by recording detailed observations of who/what/where/etc. As previewed on Page 1, we'll talk more in depth shortly about "what to record." For now, it's making the most of your immersion in the field by extensively documenting as much as you can about what you see.
  2. At the same time, following principles of "good" research design and analysis. In other words: this one "keeps you honest as a scientist" and thus helps to guard against losing your objectivity. This would include, for instance, things like having a partner-observer against whom you can bounce off your own observations -- really a reliability and validity check. It also includes use of aids such as tape recorders, video-tapes in some cases, photographs, sketches, notebooks, etc., to help ensure accuracy (vs. "hearing what you want to hear"). Finally, it can include something called a "member check". In a nutshell, a "member check" means taking back a summary of your findings and conclusions back to key informants in the field for a reality check. You are in essence asking, "Did I, as an outsider, capture your 'lived experience' accurately?" In general, having a well thought out research plan, along the lines of the "pieces" we've been learning here and in Research Design (e.g., Methodology; Population and Sampling; Instrumentation Construction and Validation; Data Collection Procedures and Data Analysis Procedures) helps to remind you that there are such research "rules" to play by,regardless of whether your study is quantitative, qualitative, or both - and thus helps guard against your own (accidental) bias, subjectivity, etc.

Open this link to look at an example of a participant-observer case study.

The Culture Of Military Organizations: A Participant-Observer Case Study
Of Cultural Diversity

II. Relative Advantages of Field-Based Participant Observation

  1. "Real" means "believable" and perhaps also "more generalizable." The classic experimental paradigm would bemoan the fact that you are out there "with no control and bundles of variables." Yet, at the same time, what better way to accurately replicate "true reality" in your study?! Odds are that because you are so closely observing and recording "real" behaviors and conditions, your findings and conclusions will inherently carry more credibility and relevance than those of the rigidly controlled, highly unrealistic laboratory. Some research design books make the point this way:

    1. Experimental designs, due to the high degree of control of extraneous variables, manipulation of one or more experimental conditions, etc., possess high internal validity. You can more precisely say "when I change X, then Y will change" and thus identify pure cause-effect relationships under such conditions -- because most, if not all, potential contaminants have been explicitly and rigidly controlled. In other words, we say they have high internal validity (credibility/believability of asserted findings). Yet, while applauding this ability to zero in on strict cause-effect relationships, researchers and especially practitioners have retorted,"That is all well and good, but my classroom (clinic, counseling center) is hardly a laboratory! Things happen that I simply can't control in the classic experimental sense -- life happens, you know?! So -- what's the practical usefulness of your findings and results to my 'real world mess?!'" In other words, low external validity (generalizability)!
    2. Field-based, "naturalistic" studies, on the other hand, are inherently so "variable-laden-and-messy" -- anything goes! You take what you get when you throw yourself in there in the field! -- that it is virtually impossible to carefully tease out those rigid X - > Y causal relationships. So we would characterize them as possessing relatively low internal validity. At the same time, though, due to this same realism, it is more likely that others with similar real-world conditions will be able to more readily apply the corresponding findings and results of our naturalistic investigation. Thus, the tradeoff here is greater external validity.

    Please open this link to look at very interesting example of a "naturalistic" study done on the web. It is a participant-observer study on the Xenaverse, "the online spaces devoted to the cult following of the syndicated television program "Xena, Warrior Princess.

    The Ballad of the Internet Nutball: Chaining Rhetorical Visions from the Margins of the Margins to the Mainstream in the Xenaverse (Rensselaer Polytechnic Institute doctoral dissertation)

    Perhaps the best way to summarize this advantage, then, is to cite yet another of our qualitative gurus, Robert K. Yin. He has reminded us that when you are doing qualitative research, you are really studying two things:

    i. The phenomenon/phenomena -- e.g., your target variable(s) of interest, such as for Dee Dee Nevelle of Sierra Vista: "the factors that constitute 'good climate' in a school"

    ii. Embedded in context -- this is so important! Unlike the sterile laboratory, you might think of this as "but those variables go with the territory!" Like it or not, Dee Dee was observing and identifying 'good climate' variables in this school, with this staff, in this town, during this time period, and so forth.

    To sum up, the immersive and carefully balanced scientific aspects of field-based participant observation let us have a good, closeup view of both of the above features which go hand in hand with any qualitative investigation.

  2. Such "close-up immersive experiences" also make it easier for the researcher to do "classic" grounded-theory exploratory/descriptive types of research. In other words, he/she knows it's OK to come to the setting without any prior theoretical models in place. By observing carefully and in detail, he/she can thereby allow potential important variables or factors to 'emerge naturally' out of such an immersion. Observation gives you more of a closeup look at these, and thus the opportunity for such 'eurekas' of 'pop-through' variables, than would, say, a one-shot, impersonal administration of an open-ended survey to a group of sample subjects. While both are qualitative in nature, the latter does not allow as much time, closeness, or opportunity to 'see and discover' the potential explanatory variables in quite the same way.

  3. The participant-observer-researcher may bring "fresh eyes" into the situation and thus notice potentially important aspects of the situation that subjects who are immersed in it might miss or take for granted. This is the old "fish out of water" phenomenon. Because of your 'newness' to the setting, you may more easily pick up on little nuances that have become comfortable old habits - and thus sublimated to the subconscious - to the subjects in the field. We've all experienced this phenomenon to some degree, I think. Silly example, but I hope it makes the point: I was in a large-group meeting once where the temperature of the room was rather cold. I say it that way because most of us somehow got subconsciously acclimated to it. Perhaps our coping mechanisms included anything from the frequently refilled coffee pot, to the "heat" generated by animated discussion regarding the issues at hand! Anyway, we had a colleague join us midday; and what laughter and surprise filled the room when that colleague instantly remarked, "Why, this room is so cold you could hang meat in it!" Seriously, things like trust levels; "underground grapevines" for communication; automatic deference to one or more individuals in a group decision setting; etc., etc. may be so entrenched in the subjects' lived experience that they don't even notice it, or would even think to tell you. Yet, with the 'fresh eyes' you bring to the situation, you may spot it outright. And if it somehow critically relates to the phenomenon/phenomena you are there to observe -- and I'll bet in most cases, it does -- then you have just made an important discovery.

  4. Related to the preceding point, there may be things that subjects are aware of, but yet are simply unwilling to talk about in an interview. Perhaps you can observe some or all of these through visual and other manifestations by immersing yourself in the field. Also, the interview setting may not allow enough time for such "iceberg-surface-level" yet potentially explanatory issues to emerge.

  5. Finally, here's a rather interesting twist on the ol' "concern for researcher bias" that is part and parcel of classic quantitative designs. Mind you, I'm not saying that isn't a legitimate concern! If anything, our earlier discussion as to the dangers of "too participant extreme" warn against sacrificing scientific scholarly objectivity. But here's perhaps a hidden benefit to having the researcher's own observations and yes, feelings, as a cross-check. Please keep in mind that the perceptions of subjects, are just that - perceptions. As we know all too well, perceptions do not necessarily match reality! Also, as reviewed in Point 3 of this list, above, those participants may honestly not "perceive" some aspect of a situation and thus not be aware of it, due to their extensive immersion. And as Point 4 has told us, they may be uncomfortable, embarrassed or otherwise unwilling to share candidly in some cases, even if they are aware. So here again, rather than being a 'bias,' you as an outsider may bring 'fresh eyes' and a 'clean' perspective with which to more accurately 'capture' a wider range of field-based variables and factors.

 


Once you have finished you should:

Go back to Participant and Observer

E-mail M. Dereshiwsky at statcatmd@aol.com
Call M. Dereshiwsky at (520) 523-1892


NAU

Copyright © 1999 Northern Arizona University
ALL RIGHTS RESERVED